Week 1
Week 1 part 1
Key Concepts, References, and Readings
NOTE: All these references and readings are optional.
Video 1: Introduction to Teaching Online
Key Concepts
-
Research has shown that students can learn even better online than they can in the traditional face-to-face classroom. Teaching online does NOT have to mean reduced effectiveness.
-
Online teaching that emphasizes active practice while tightening up lecture content appears to be highly efficient.
References
General references
-
Oakley B, Rogowsky B, Sejnowski T. Uncommon Sense Teaching: Penguin Random House; 2021. See in particular Chapter 9: Online Teaching with Personality and Flair.
-
Mayer, R and Fiorella, L. The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning. 3rd ed: Cambridge University Press, 2021.
-
Prince, M, et al. “Active Student Engagement in Online STEM Classes: Approaches and Recommendations.” Advances in Engineering Education 8, (2020).
-
Shachar, M., & Neumann, Y. (2010). “Twenty years of research on the academic performance differences between traditional and distance learning: Summative meta–analysis and trend examination.” MERLOT Journal of Online Learning and Teaching, 6(2).
Terry and Barb, writing, researching, and thinking about world of teaching and learning online
-
Chen K-Z, Oakley B. “Redeveloping a global MOOC to be more locally relevant: design-based research.” International Journal of Educational Technology in Higher Education. 2020;17(1):1-22.
-
Oakley, B. A., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2019). “What we learned from creating one of the world’s most popular MOOCs.” npj Science of Learning, 4, 1-7.
-
Oakley B. Mindshift: Break Through Obstacles to Learning and Discover Your Hidden Potential: Penguin-Random House; 2017. See in particular chapters 11 and 12 on MOOCs and creating online courses.
-
Oakley B, Poole D, Nestor M. “Creating a sticky MOOC.” OLC Journal of Online Learning. 2016;20(1):1-12.
Students can learn as well or even better online
- See pages 6-7 of Joyner, D and Isbell, C. The Distributed Classroom: MIT Press, 2021 for a contextualized discussion, citing the research literature, of how students learn better online. We quote here:
“To illustrate the range of possible differences [between online and traditional education], we take the example of an online undergraduate class we launched in January 2017 at Georgia Tech. In developing this class, we paid close attention to the research showing that learning outcomes often tend to lag in online compared to traditional classes. We wanted to ensure that the online class—CS1301: Introduction to Computing—could promise comparable learning gains to the traditional version of the same curriculum before rolling it out to a larger audience. The class we produced ended up turning out students who learned as much as or more than students in a traditional version of the class.8 Other experiments at MIT and Carnegie Mellon have found similar results.9 The class has been offered every semester (fifteen terms total) since, totaling over three thousand course completers for credit, and has also been launched as a MOOC; over ten thousand students have completed a MOOC version of the course. This scale was possible only because of the favorable learning gains we observed in our experiments over the first couple of years of delivering the course: without evidence of the learning outcomes, we would have been reluctant to expand the course so heavily.
“These results run counter to an influential thread of research about online education, where the finding has been that outcomes suffer in online environments compared to traditional environments. In response to this result, some have argued that students in selective and prestigious research institutions like MIT, Georgia Tech, and Carnegie Mellon are themselves better prepared to succeed in online classes; they possess the discipline and self-regulation skills necessary to monitor their own progress with limited external structures ensuring their continued engagement.10 Much of the research finding poorer outcomes in online classes comes from community colleges and MOOC providers, and so some argue that the achievement difference is due to differences in the students. Online classes, then, could contribute to a widening of the achievement gap as they allow already well-educated students to move forward even faster based on their ability to succeed in more flexibly-available online courses.
“Others—ourselves included—pose a different explanation. These large research institutions have and are devoting significant resources to developing online initiatives. When David developed our online Introduction to Computing class, we spent a full year writing the textbook, filming the lectures, and developing the initial assessments. We had a team of nearly a dozen people supporting David, including video producers, textbook copyeditors, project managers, technologists, and teaching assistants; in many ways, we had far more support than even traditional face-to-face classes have, before we even consider David’s own prior experience teaching online. Our online master’s-level courses are similarly developed by teams of professors, teaching assistants, instructional technologists, and project managers using world-class facilities. It is perhaps unsurprising that such a large investment of resources creates an educational experience leading to superior learning outcomes.”
-
For a full exploration of the learning outcomes of the two versions of the class, see David Joyner, “Toward CS1 at Scale: Building and Testing a MOOC-for-Credit Candidate,” in Proceedings of the Fifth Annual ACM Conference on Learning at Scale (New York: ACM, 2018); and David Joyner and Melinda McDaniel, “Replicating and Unraveling Performance and Behavioral Differences between an Online and a Traditional CS Course,” in Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Global Computing Education, 157–163 (New York: ACM, 2019).
-
For information on Carnegie Mellon’s results, see: M. Lovett, O. Meyer, and C. Thille, “The Open Learning Initiative: Measuring the Effectiveness of the OLI Statistics Course in Accelerating Student Learning,” Journal of Interactive Media in Education (2008). Results from MIT’s similar exploration can be found at Piotr F. Mitros, Khurram K. Afridi, Gerald J. Sussman, Chris J. Terman, Jacob K. White, Lyla Fischer, and Anant Agarwal, “Teaching Electronic Circuits Online: Lessons from MITx’s 6.002 x on edX,” in Proceedings of the 2013 IEEE International Symposium on Circuits and Systems, 2763–2766 (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE, 2013).
-
For more on the relationship between self-regulation and success in online classes, see Richard Lynch and Myron Dembo, “The Relationship between Self-Regulation and Online Learning in a Blended Learning Context,” International Review of Research in Open and Distributed Learning 5, no. 2 (2004); Rachel L. Bradley, Blaine L. Browne, and Heather M. Kelley, “Examining the Influence of Self-Efficacy and Self-Regulation in Online Learning,” College Student Journal 51, no. 4 (2017): 518–530; and Heather Kauffman, “A Review of Predictive Factors of Student Success in and Satisfaction with Online Learning,” Research in Learning Technology 23 (2015).
-
Shachar, M., & Neumann, Y. (2010). “Twenty years of research on the academic performance differences between traditional and distance learning: Summative meta–analysis and trend examination.” MERLOT Journal of Online Learning and Teaching, 6(2).
-
Sun, A., & Chen, X. (2016). “Online education and its effective practice: A research review.” Journal of Information Technology Education: Research, 15, 157–190.
Video 2: Be Wary of Checkbox Advice in Creating Online Courses—The Expertise Reversal Effect and Schemas
Key Concepts
-
The expertise reversal effect is observed when certain teaching methods can impede the learning of already-skilled learners.
-
Learning involves making connections between neurons in long-term memory.
-
A set of connected neurons involving our knowledge on a topic or in a subject area form what’s called a “schema.”
-
Wading through a lot of material just to make a tiny adjustment in a schema is something that students (as well as we teachers) prefer to avoid.
References
Schemas
-
Babichev, A and Dabaghian, YA. “Topological schemas of memory spaces.” Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 12, (2018): 27.
-
Frankland, PW, et al. “The neurobiological foundation of memory retrieval.” Nat Neurosci 22, no. 10 (2019): 1576-1585.
-
Ghosh, VE and Gilboa, A. “What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature.” Neuropsychologia 53, (2014): 104-114.
-
Gilboa, A and Marlatte, H. “Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory.” Trends Cogn Sci 21, no. 8 (2017): 618-631.
-
Josselyn, SA and Tonegawa, S. “Memory engrams: Recalling the past and imagining the future.” Science 367, no. 6473 (2020): eaaw4325.
-
Richards, BA, et al. “Patterns across multiple memories are identified over time.” Nat Neurosci 17, no. 7 (2014): 981-986.
-
Tse, D, et al. “Schema-dependent gene activation and memory encoding in neocortex.” Science 333, no. 6044 (2011): 891-895.
Expertise reversal effect
-
Armougum, A, et al. “Expertise reversal effect: Cost of generating new schemas.” _Computers in Human Behavio_r 111, (2020): 106406.
-
Kalyuga, S and Sweller, J. “Cognitive load and expertise reversal.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance, edited by K. A. Ericsson, R. R. Hoffman, A. Kozbelt and A. M. Williams: Cambridge University Press, 2018.
-
Sweller, J, et al. “The expertise reversal effect.” Educational Psychologist 38, no. 1 (2003): 23-31.
Video 3: Low-stakes Onboarding Quizzes and Avoiding the Horrors of Goodhart’s Law
Key Concepts
-
A low stakes on-boarding quiz is a great way to help students learn the most important aspects of your course and your teaching style.
-
If you are teaching a “flipped” class, you may wish to reduce the average scores for the online quizzes to the average scores for the in-person quizzes, since it can be easier to control for cheating in a traditional, in-person classroom setting.
-
Goodhart’s Law states that “When a measure becomes a target, it ceases to be a good measure.” This law reminds us that it can be all-too-easy to fall into a “checkbox” approach that seems to satisfy all the requirements, but in reality, produces a poor online learning experience for students.
References
Expertise reversal effect
-
Armougum, A, et al. “Expertise reversal effect: Cost of generating new schemas.” _Computers in Human Behavio_r 111, (2020): 106406.
-
Kalyuga, S and Sweller, J. “Cognitive load and expertise reversal.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance, edited by K. A. Ericsson, R. R. Hoffman, A. Kozbelt and A. M. Williams: Cambridge University Press, 2018.
-
Sweller, J, et al. “The expertise reversal effect.” Educational Psychologist 38, no. 1 (2003): 23-31.
The value of practice tests
- Adesope, OO, et al. “Rethinking the use of tests: A meta-analysis of practice testing.” Review of Educational Research 87, no. 3 (2017): 659-701.
Goodhart’s Law
- The Wikipedia article on Goodhart’s Law provides a good general overview and further references.
Video 4: Create a Quick Walkthrough of Your Course
Key Concepts
- Creating a screencast walkthrough can help onboard your students more quickly and easily into your course.
References
-
Berdahl, L. (2021, August 27). “How to get students to read your syllabus.” University Affairs.
-
Dachner, AM and Saxton, BM. “If you don’t care, then why should I? The influence of instructor commitment on student satisfaction and commitment.” Journal of Management Education 39, no. 5 (2015): 549-571.
-
Fitton, T. “Touring the online learning environment: An exploration using screencast-o-matic.” European Journal of Open, Distance and E-learning, (2018).
-
Hartmann, T, et al. “Spatial presence theory: State of the art and challenges ahead.” In Immersed in Media: Telepresence Theory, Measurement & Technology, edited by Matthew Lombard, Frank Biocca, Jonathan Freeman, Wijnand Ijsselsteijn and Rachel J. Schaevitz, 115-135. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2015.
Week 1 part 2
Key Concepts, References, and Readings
NOTE: All these references and readings are optional
Video 5: Schemas of Identity Can Provide Motivation—or Demotivation
Key Concepts
-
To get students to engage with one another, you must first get them to engage with you.
-
Taken together, all of our schemas help provide an overarching sense involving who we are—our identity. Our identity provides fundamental motivations for our actions.
-
Our job as teachers is to help students develop as well as change their schemas.
References
- Costa, K. 99 Tips for Creating Simple and Sustainable Educational Videos: A Guide for Online Teachers and Flipped Classes: Stylus Publishing, LLC, 2020.
Students focus (first) on videos
-
Breslow, L, et al. “Studying learning in the worldwide classroom research into edX’s first MOOC.” RPA Journal 8, (2013): 13-25.
-
Ekstrom, A. “Navigation in Virtual Space: Psychological and Neural Aspects,” In: Koob G.F., Le Moal M. and Thompson R.F. (eds.) Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience, volume 2, pp. 286–293 Oxford: Academic Press
-
Hsin, W-J and Cigas, J. “Short videos improve student learning in online education.” Journal of Computing Sciences in Colleges 28, no. 5 (2013): 253-259.
-
Kizilcec, RF, et al. “Deconstructing disengagement: Analyzing learner subpopulations in massive open online courses.” In Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Learning Analytics and Knowledge, edited by D Suthers, K Verbert, E Duval and X Ochoa, 170-179. Leuven, Belgium, 2013.
-
Oakley, BA and Sejnowski, TJ. “What we learned from creating one of the world’s most popular MOOCs.” npj Science of Learning, 4, Article 7 (2019): 1-7.
-
Seaton, DT, et al. “Who does what in a massive open online course?” Commun ACM 57, no. 4 (2014): 58-65.
Identity (versus goal-based) habit formation
-
Melnikoff, DE, et al. “Editorial: On the nature and scope of habits and model-free control.” Frontiers in Psychology 12, (2021): 760841.
-
Oyserman, D, et al. “Identity-based motivation and health.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93, no. 6 (2007): 1011. This paper is a classic with relation to identity-based motivation.
-
Oyserman, D, et al. “An identity-based motivation framework for self-regulation.” Psychological Inquiry 28, no. 2-3 (2017): 139-147.
-
Oyserman, D. Pathways to Success through Identity-Based Motivation: Oxford University Press, USA, 2015.
-
Williamson, LZ and Wilkowski, BM. “What we repeatedly do: Evaluating the determinants and consequences of habit enactment during daily goal‐pursuit.” British Journal of Psychology 113, no. 1 (2022): 1-24. This paper emphasizes the importance of tiny habitual actions that support long-term goals.
Video 6: The Imposter Syndrome in Online Teaching—Meet Television Star Audrey Lawrence
Key Concepts
- Students can change their innermost core beliefs about themselves—the schemas underlying their identity—through online learning.
References
-
Audrey Lawrence: https://www.audreylawrence.org/
-
Woolley, K and Fishbach, A. “Motivating personal growth by seeking discomfort.” Psychological Science 33, no. 4 (2022): 510-523.
Week 2 part 1
Key Concepts, References, and Readings
NOTE: All these references and readings are optional
- Chapter 6 of Uncommon Sense Teaching is especially helpful in providing helpful information related to procedural system, habit-based learning.
Video 1: On (and Off) Camera Habits and the Declarative-Procedural Learning Systems: Why Knowing and Doing are Not the Same Thing
Key Concepts
-
Two main pathways are used to deposit or tweak links in our neural schemas: The declarative and the procedural pathways.
-
The declarative pathway takes our conscious thoughts from the front of the brain through the hippocampus into long-term memory.
-
The procedural pathway of learning flows through the basal ganglia. We are not conscious of what we are doing when we are using the procedural sets of links. Still, these types of links underpin our ability to do highly sophisticated activities, like solve a Rubik’s Cube or speak our native language.
-
Procedurally-laid links underpin our habits. They can be hard to change.
References
-
For general overview of the declarative (Chapter 3) and procedural (Chapter 6) systems and how they relate to long-term memory, see Oakley, B, Rogowski, B, Sejnowski, T, Uncommon Sense Teaching: Penguin Random House, 2021, and the references therein. Chapter 9: Online teaching with personality and flair.
-
For references related to schemas, see Week 1, Video 2.
Transactional Distance
-
Shearer, RL and Park, E. “The theory of transactional distance.” In Open and Distance Education Theory Revisited, 31-38: Springer, 2019.
-
Stein, DS, et al. “Bridging the transactional distance gap in online learning environments.” The American Journal of Distance Education 19, no. 2 (2005): 105-118.
-
Moore, MG. “Theory of transactional distance.” Theoretical Principles of Distance Education 1, (1993): 22-38.
Habit
We will get more into habit later this week, so look there for many more references on this topic. In the meantime, two good popular books about habit include:
-
Duhigg, C. The Power of Habit. NY: Random House, 2012.
-
Wood, W. Good Habits, Bad Habits: The Science of Making Positive Changes that Stick: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2019. Dr. Wood is an important researcher in this area, so her book is particularly worthwhile.
The Difficulties of “Unlearning”
-
Allaire-Duquette, G, et al. “An fMRI study of scientists with a Ph. D. in physics confronted with naive ideas in science.” npj Science of Learning 6, no. 1 (2021): 1-12. This paper observes: “Results suggest that naive ideas are likely to persist, even after completing a Ph.D. Advanced experts may still rely on high order executive functions like inhibitory control to overcome naive ideas when the context requires it.”
-
Dunbar, K, et al. “Do naïve theories ever go away? Using brain and behavior to understand changes in concepts.” In Carnegie Mellon Symposia on Cognition. Thinking with Data, edited by M. C. Lovett and P. Shah, 193-206: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, 2007.
-
Halloun, IA and Hestenes, D. “The initial knowledge state of college physics students.” American Journal of Physics 53, no. 11 (1985): 1043-1055.
-
NOVA. “The science of smart: How to unlearn mistaken ideas.” PBS (2013).
-
Scherr, RE, et al. “The challenge of changing deeply held student beliefs about the relativity of simultaneity.” American Journal of Physics 70, no. 12 (2002): 1238-1248.
-
Shtulman, A and Valcarcel, J. “Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant earlier intuitions.” Cognition 124, no. 2 (2012): 209-215.
Video 2: Lights, Camera, Action—Oops!
Key Concepts
- Your microphone and ambient environment, camera, camera positioning, and lights have an important effect on your ability to teach well online. The equipment you choose to use is probably the place where the smallest decisions can have the biggest impact.
References
-
Coursera has put together an awesome listing of lighting, microphones, cameras, recording and editing software and the like that you can find here: bit.ly/Coursera-Home-Production-Guidance.
-
Schneider, S., et al. (2016). “Decorative pictures and emotional design in multimedia learning.” Learning and Instruction 44: 65-73.
Video 3: “You’re Ready for your Close-Up… Wait, Too Close!”
Key Concepts
-
Your audio quality is much more important than your video quality when you are teaching online.
-
Your visual background can either be simple or can provide hints for your students about your likes and interests.
-
Just before going on camera, recheck your:
-
Lighting
-
Camera framing (common errors are the Frankenstein and gopher effects, or having the head too close to the camera)
-
Sound (eliminate the noise of fans if possible)
-
References
-
Coursera’s listing of lighting, microphones, cameras, recording and editing software: bit.ly/Coursera-Home-Production-Guidance.
-
“Here’s Why Movie Dialogue Has Gotten More Difficult To Understand (And Three Ways To Fix It),” by Ben Pearson, Slashfilm, 2022.
Video 4: Breaking Bad Habits When It Comes to Teaching Online—and in Everyday Life
Key Concepts
-
The best way to change a bad habit—like how you position yourself on camera—is to bring the change you want to conscious awareness, for example, using reminder post-it notes.
-
Retrieval practice (“recall”) and spaced repetition help to lay new declarative sets of links and, over time, turn those declarative links into procedural links.
References
- See the references for Week 2, Video 1
Week 2 part 2
NOTE: All these references and readings are optional
Video 5: Talk to the Hand—the Power of Gesture to Help Form Mental Models
Key Concepts
-
Mental models are the thoughts we are holding in working memory. We develop mental models related to concepts, ideas or events we are trying to follow or understand.
-
Our gestures and movements can help us develop mental models because they activate a rich network of neurons—our schemas—of remembered experiences that allows us to think more deeply about the ideas we are considering.
-
When people watch another person move or gesture, their neural networks and schemas are subtly activated to mirror the person they are watching. This is called the “mirror rule.” These mirrored activations help with both mimicking and the development of a mental model.
-
Whenever you have a success, your brain learns from it and programs that learning into your neural networks to help you repeat that success. This has been termed the “success rule.”
General References
-
Al-Diban, S. “Mental models.” In Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning, edited by Norbert M. Seel, 2200-2204. Boston, MA: Springer US, 2012.
-
Beaubien, R and Parrish, S. The Great Mental Models, Latticework Publishing Inc., 2018.
-
Bhalwankar, R and Treur, J. “In control of your instructor: Modeling learner-controlled mental model learning.” In _Mental Models and Their Dynamics, Adaptation, and Contro_l, 209-253: Springer, 2022.
-
Chartrand, TL, et al. “Beyond the perception-behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry.” In The New Unconscious, edited by Ran R. Hassin, James S. Uleman and John A. Bargh, 334-361: Oxford University Press, 2005.
-
Hickok, G. The Myth of Mirror Neurons: The Real Neuroscience of Communication and Cognition: WW Norton & Company, 2014.
-
Churchland, PS. Braintrust: What Neuroscience Tells Us About Morality: Princeton University Press, 2011. See in particular Chapters 5 and 5. As Churchland notes: “A neuron, though computationally complex, is just a neuron. It is not an intelligent homunculus. If a neural network represents something complex, such as an intention [to insult], it must have the right input and be in the right place in the neural circuitry to do that.” Chapter 6, page 142.
-
Guo, PJ, et al. “How video production affects student engagement: an empirical study of MOOC videos.” In Proceedings of the First ACM Conference on Learning@Scale Conference, 41-50. Atlanta, GA USA, 2014.
-
Jones, NA, et al. “Mental models: an interdisciplinary synthesis of theory and methods.” Ecology and Society 16, no. 1 (2011). [This is a major explanatory paper.]
-
Mayer, RE. “Evidence-based principles for how to design effective instructional videos.” Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 10, no. 2 (2021): 229-240. See in particular his discussion of voice (use appealing human voice) and embodiment (display gesturing instructor),
-
Mayer, R and Fiorella, L. The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning. 3rd ed: Cambridge University Press, 2021.
-
Noah, T, et al. “When both the original study and Its failed replication are correct: Feeling observed eliminates the facial-feedback effect.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 114, no. 5 (2018): 657.
-
Paley, Christopher, “Smiling does make you happier – under carefully controlled conditions: The idea that smiling changes the way we perceive things seemed like another casualty of social psychology’s replication crisis – but something more interesting was going on,” The Guardian, 2018. This easy-to-read popular article does a good job summarizing the ebbs and flows of research in this area.
-
Pontis, Shiela, “Making sense of mental models in information design,” blog post, July 25, 2021. This post gives a nice general overview of how information designers think about mental models and schemas, and just how differently people can view these terms, depending on their background and training.
-
Prince, M, et al. “Active Student Engagement in Online STEM Classes: Approaches and Recommendations.” Advances in Engineering Education 8, (2020).
-
Rau, MA and Herder, T. “Under which conditions are physical versus virtual representations effective? Contrasting conceptual and embodied mechanisms of learning.” Journal of Educational Psychology, (2021).
-
Radvansky, GA and Zacks, JM. “Event perception.” Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 2, no. 6 (2011): 608-620. This interesting paper describes how mental models encompass both “system” and “event” models. In our MOOC, we often describe the “event model” aspect of mental models, in line with the descriptions in this paper. But do keep in mind that other neuroscientists can and do explain mental models differently than what Radvansky and Zacks have explained in their paper.
-
Treur, J. “Mental models in the brain: On context-dependent neural correlates of mental models.” Cognitive Systems Research 69, (2021): 83-90.
-
Treur, J. “How do mental models actually exist in the brain: On context-dependent neural correlates of mental models.” In Mental Models and Their Dynamics, Adaptation, and Control, 409-426: Springer, 2022.
-
Varga, N, et al. “Schema, inference, and memory.” In Oxford Handbook of Human Memory, edited by Michael J. Kahana and Anthony D. Wagner: Oxford University Press, 2022.
-
van Ments, L and Treur, J. “Reflections on dynamics, adaptation and control: A cognitive architecture for mental models.” Cognitive Systems Research 70, (2021): 1-9.
-
Wade, L. “Experimental evidence for expectation-driven linguistic convergence.” Language, (2022).
-
Zacks, Jeffrey. Flicker: Your Brain on Movies. Oxford University Press. 2014. (See especially Chapter 1.)
-
Zajonc, Robert. “Attitudinal effects of mere exposure.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 9 (1968): 1-27.
-
Zwaan RA. “Effect of genre expectations on text comprehension.” J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1994, 20:920–933. (As noted in Radvansky and Zacks, 2011.)
Gesture
-
Dargue, N, et al. “When our hands help us understand: A meta-analysis into the effects of gesture on comprehension.” Psychol Bull 145, no. 8 (2019): 765-784.
-
Davis, RO, et al. “Does a pedagogical agent’s gesture frequency assist advanced foreign language users with learning declarative knowledge?” International Journal of Educational Technology in Higher Education 18, no. 1 (2021).
-
Davis, RO, et al. “The effects of virtual human gesture frequency and reduced video speed on satisfaction and learning outcomes.” Educational Technology Research and Development 69, no. 5 (2021): 2331-2352.
-
Halvorson, KM, et al. “The role of motor context in the beneficial effects of hand gesture on memory.” Atten Percept Psychophys 81, no. 7 (2019): 2354-2364.
-
Kendon, A. Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance: Cambridge University Press, 2004. (This book is a classic that helped launch the field of gesture studies.)
-
Kita, S, et al. “How do gestures influence thinking and speaking? The gesture-for-conceptualization hypothesis.” Psychological Review 124, no. 3 (2017): 245.
-
Pan, Y, et al. “Instructor-learner body coupling reflects instruction and learning.” NPJ Sci Learn 7, no. 1 (2022): 15.
-
Ping, R, et al. “Teaching Stereoisomers Through Gesture, Action, and Mental Imagery.” Chemistry Education Research and Practice, (2022). This intriguing paper also rightly notes that “gesture is not necessarily the superior instructional choice for all concepts and training paradigms.”
-
Schneider, S., et al. (2022). “The impact of video lecturers’ nonverbal communication on learning–An experiment on gestures and facial expressions of pedagogical agents.” Computers & Education 176: 104350.
-
Turella, L and Lingnau, A. “Neural correlates of grasping.” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, (2014).
Video 6: Diving Deeper into Mental Models
Key Concepts
-
Mental models are like a flock of neural birds continually rearranging themselves in the air as you attempt to grasp a concept or understand a situation.
-
If you develop a mental model that is important enough to you, and you gain enough experience with it, your mental model can gradually be integrated into your neural schemas.
References: How birds (not to mention neurons) “flock” together
-
Chen, D, et al. “Inferring causal relationship in coordinated flight of pigeon flocks.” Chaos: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Nonlinear Science 29, no. 11 (2019): 113118.
-
Chung, S and Abbott, L. “Neural population geometry: An approach for understanding biological and artificial neural networks.” Current Opinion in Neurobiology 70, (2021): 137-144.
-
Cordova, NI, et al. “Focusing on what matters: Modulation of the human hippocampus by relational attention.” Hippocampus, (2019).
-
Franklin, NT, et al. “Structured event memory: A neuro-symbolic model of event cognition.” Psychological Review 127, no. 3 (2020): 327.
-
Friederici, P. “How a flock of birds can fly and move together.” Audubon Magazine (2009).
-
Gilboa, A and Marlatte, H. “Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory.” Trends Cogn Sci 21, no. 8 (2017): 618-631.
-
Kurby, CA and Zacks, JM. “Priming of movie content is modulated by event boundaries.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, (2021).
-
Musall, S, et al. “Single-trial neural dynamics are dominated by richly varied movements.” Nature Neuroscience 22, no. 10 (2019): 1677-1686.
-
Nieh, EH, et al. “Geometry of abstract learned knowledge in the hippocampus.” Nature 595, no. 7865 (2021): 80-84. This especially-intriguing paper found that the hippocampus seems to perform “a general computation—the creation of task-specific low-dimensional manifolds that contain a geometric representation of learned knowledge.” In other words, the hippocampus can be thought of as where the neural “flock” flies around as it is attempting to grasp an event model.
-
Pao, GM, et al. “Experimentally testable whole brain manifolds that recapitulate behavior.” arXiv preprint arXiv:2106.10627, (2021).
-
Sarma, AA, et al. “Internal feedback in biological control: Architectures and examples.” arXiv preprint arXiv:2110.05029, (2021).
-
Sekeres, MJ, et al. “The hippocampus and related neocortical structures in memory transformation.” Neurosci Lett 680, (2018): 39-53. The nomenclature of what we call “event models” can vary substantially, depending on what theories are being followed and what varying factors are involved. As Sekeres et all note with respect to the concept of episodic memory: “Episodic memories are multifaceted and malleable, capable of being transformed with time and experience at both the neural level and psychological level. At the neural level, episodic memories are transformed from being dependent on the hippocampus to becoming represented in neocortical structures, such as the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and back again, while at the psychological level, detailed, perceptually rich memories, are transformed to ones retaining only the gist of an experience or a schema related to it. Trace Transformation Theory (TTT) initially proposed that neural and psychological transformations are linked and proceed in tandem… At the heart of the updated TTT lies the long axis of the hippocampus whose functional differentiation and connectivity to neocortex make it a hub for memory formation and transformation. The posterior hippocampus, connected to perceptual and spatial representational systems in posterior neocortex, supports fine, perceptually rich, local details of memories; the anterior hippocampus, connected to conceptual systems in anterior neocortex, supports coarse, global representations that constitute the gist of a memory. Notable among the anterior neocortical structures is the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) which supports representation of schemas that code for common aspects of memories across different episodes. Linking the aHPC with mPFC is the entorhinal cortex (EC) which conveys information needed for the interaction/translation between gist and schemas. Thus, the long axis of the hippocampus, mPFC and EC provide the representational gradient, from fine to coarse and from perceptual to conceptual, that can implement processes implicated in memory transformation. Each of these representations of an episodic memory can co-exist and be in dynamic flux as they interact with one another throughout the memory’s lifetime, going from detailed to schematic and possibly back again, all mediated by corresponding changes in neural representation.”
-
Steinmetz, NA, et al. “Distributed coding of choice, action and engagement across the mouse brain.” Nature 576, no. 7786 (2019): 266-273.
-
Sugihara, G, et al. “Detecting causality in complex ecosystems.” Science 338, no. 6106 (2012): 496-500.
-
Puiu, T. “How do birds flock together? Birds of a feather flock together… but how do they decide where to go and who to follow?” ZME Science (2019).
-
Vyas, S, et al. “Computation through neural population dynamics.” Annual Review of Neuroscience 43, (2020): 249.
Video 7: Catching Continuity Errors at the Movies—How Mental Models Arise
Key Concepts
-
Mental models involve what is being seen, heard, or otherwise experienced. But they also make predictions even as they draw on memories (schemas) of previous experiences.
-
Virtually everything we do as teachers is done to help students build mental models and schemas in STUDENT’S brains that are similar to the mental models and schemas in OUR brains.
References
- See references for Week 2, videos 4 and 5. For the inspiration behind this video, we are particularly indebted to Dr. Jeffrey Zacks’ discussions in his book Flicker: Your Brain on Movies. Oxford University Press. 2014.
Video 8: Predicting Effective Online Strategies—Insight from Mental Models & More
Key Concepts
-
In general, it can be helpful to ask students to make predictions in order to activate prior knowledge within their schemas and help them to begin the process of making a mental model.
-
Theoretical techniques that can work well in traditional classrooms don’t necessarily transfer to the online world.
References
-
Brod, G. “Predicting as a learning strategy.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, (2021): 1-9.
-
Lang, JM. Small Teaching: Everyday Lessons from the Science of Learning: John Wiley & Sons, 2021.
-
Seabrooke, T, et al. “The benefits of impossible tests: Assessing the role of error-correction in the pretesting effect.” Memory & Cognition 50, no. 2 (2022): 296-311.
Video 9: How Long Should Online Videos Be? More Insight from Mental Models
Key Concepts:
-
Although shorter videos are in general better, the maximum length of good online videos is thought to be in the range of 12-20 minutes, not 6 minutes.
-
Inserting indicators that help frame key concepts can improve memory for that key concept.
References
-
Guo, PJ, et al. “How video production affects student engagement: an empirical study of MOOC videos.” In Proceedings of the First ACM Conference on Learning@Scale Conference, 41-50. Atlanta, GA USA, 2014.
-
Franklin, NT, et al. “Structured event memory: A neuro-symbolic model of event cognition.” Psychological Review 127, no. 3 (2020): 327. If you’re interested in more detail about what an event might actually consist of, in this paper, Franklin and his colleagues, including the Zelig-like Dr. Zacks, note: “Humans spontaneously organize a continuous experience into discrete events and use the learned structure of these events to generalize and organize memory. We introduce the Structured Event Memory (SEM) model of event cognition, which accounts for human abilities in event segmentation, memory, and generalization. SEM is derived from a probabilistic generative model of event dynamics defined over structured symbolic scenes. By embedding symbolic scene representations in a vector space and parametrizing the scene dynamics in this continuous space, SEM combines the advantages of structured and neural network approaches to high-level cognition. Using probabilistic reasoning over this generative model, SEM can infer event boundaries, learn event schemata, and use event knowledge to reconstruct past experience. We show that SEM can scale up to high dimensional input spaces, producing human-like event segmentation for naturalistic video data, and accounts for a wide array of memory phenomena.”
-
Kay, J. S., Lambe, C., Nolan, T. J., Grello, T. M., & Breitzman, A. (2019, June). “Apples to apples: Differences in viewer retention when longer content is chopped into smaller bites.” In Proceedings of the Sixth (2019) ACM Conference on Learning@ Scale (pp. 1-7).
-
Kurby, CA and Zacks, JM. “Priming of movie content is modulated by event boundaries.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, (2021).
-
Lagerstrom, L, et al. “The myth of the six-minute rule: Student engagement with online videos.” In 122nd ASEE Annual Conference, pp. 14-17. Seattle, WA, 2015.
-
Mayer, RE. “Evidence-based principles for how to design effective instructional videos.” Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 10, no. 2 (2021): 229-240.
-
Sargent, JQ, et al. “Event segmentation ability uniquely predicts event memory.” Cognition 129, no. 2 (2013): 241-255.
-
Slemmons, K., Anyanwu, K., Hames, J., Grabski, D., Mlsna, J., Simkins, E., & Cook, P. (2018). “The impact of video length on learning in a middle-level flipped science setting: Implications for diversity inclusion.” Journal of Science Education and Technology, 27(5), 469-479.
-
Sweller, J, et al. Cognitive Load Theory. New York: Springer-Verlag, 2011.
-
Sweller, J, et al. “Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later.” Educational Psychology Review 31, no. 2 (2019): 261-292.
week 3
week 3 Part 1
NOTE: All these references and readings are optional
- Chapters 1 and 9 of Uncommon Sense Teaching are especially helpful in providing helpful information related to this material.
Week 3, Lesson 1: Retrieval Practice, Mental Models, and Schemas
Video 1: Today’s Online Learners Typically Have No Time to Waste
Key Concepts
-
Today’s online students often expect and benefit from tightly planned teaching that focuses on quickly conveying the key ideas.
-
We can help our students by teaching them about the value of retrieval practice and expecting them to use retrieval practice in their private study sessions between classes.
-
Retrieval practice is good, not just for relatively simple facts, but to help students gain a deeper conceptual understanding of the materials.
References
-
Agarwal, P. K. and P. Bain. Powerful Teaching: Unleash the Science of Learning: Jossey-Bass, 2019. This book is a wonderful, highly readable resource for incorporating retrieval practice into your teaching.
-
Agarwal, P. K., J. R. Finley, N. S. Rose and H. L. Roediger, 3rd. “Benefits from retrieval practice are greater for students with lower working memory capacity.” Memory 25, no. 6 (2017): 764-771.
-
Antony, JW, et al. “Retrieval as a fast route to memory consolidation.” Trends Cogn Sci 21, no. 8 (2017): 573-576.
-
Bjork, Robert A. “Being suspicious of the sense of ease and undeterred by the sense of difficulty: Looking back at Schmidt and Bjork (1992).” Perspectives on Psychological Science 13, no. 2 (2018): 146-148.
-
Bjork, EL and Bjork, RA. “Chapter 5: Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning.” Psychology and the Real World: Essays Illustrating Fundamental Contributions to Society, 2011.
-
Carvalho, PF, et al. “Self-regulated spacing in a massive open online course is related to better learning.” Npj Science of Learning 5, no. 2 (2020). This important study observes: “We found that, overall, distributing study across multiple sessions—increasing spacing—was related to increased performance on end-of-unit quizzes… Spacing benefits … were largest for the lower-ability students and for those students who were less likely to complete activities. These results suggest that spaced study might work as a buffer, improving performance for low ability students and those who do not engage in active practices.”
-
Charo, R, et al. “Self-regulation of learning and MOOC retention.” Computers in Human Behavior, (2020): 106423.
-
Ericsson, KA, et al. The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance. 2nd ed.: Cambridge University Press, 2018.
-
Lyle, Keith B, Campbell R Bego, Robin F Hopkins, Jeffrey L Hieb and Patricia AS Ralston. “How the amount and spacing of retrieval practice affect the short-and long-term retention of mathematics knowledge.” Educational Psychology Review, (2019): 1-19.
-
Karpicke, J. D. and J. R. Blunt. “Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping.” Science 331, no. 6018 (2011): 772-775.
-
Karpicke, Jeffrey D and Phillip J Grimaldi. “Retrieval-based learning: A perspective for enhancing meaningful learning.” Educational Psychology Review 24, no. 3 (2012): 401-418.
-
Koriat, Asher and Robert A Bjork. “Illusions of competence in monitoring one’s knowledge during study.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31, no. 2 (2005): 187–194.
-
Nielsen, M. “Using spaced repetition systems to see through a piece of mathematics.” Cognitive Medium (2019).
-
O’Day, Garrett M. and Jeffrey D. Karpicke. “Comparing and combining retrieval practice and concept mapping.” Journal of Educational Psychology Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000486, (2021), 113 (5), 986–997.
-
Oxford Handbook of Human Memory, Volume 1 & Volume 2. (2022; partially complete and available online).
-
Pan, SC and Bjork, RA. “Chapter 11.3 Acquiring an accurate mental model of human learning: Towards an owner’s manual.” In Oxford Handbook of Memory, Vol. II: Applications, in press.
-
Rawson, Katherine A and John Dunlosky. “Optimizing schedules of retrieval practice for durable and efficient learning: How much is enough?” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 140, no. 3 (2011): 283.
-
Roediger, Henry L and Andrew C Butler. “The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, no. 1 (2011): 20-27.
-
Smith, Amy M., Victoria A. Floerke and Ayanna K. Thomas. “Retrieval practice protects memory against acute stress.” Science 354, no. 6315 (2016).
-
Upadhyay, U, et al. “Large-scale randomized experiments reveals that machine learning-based instruction helps people memorize more effectively.” NPJ Sci Learn 6, no. 1 (2021): 26.
Video 2: What on Earth Do Retrieval Practice and Spaced Repetition Have to Do with Mental Models? Or Good Online Teaching?
Key Concepts
-
Retrieval practice with spaced repetition creates and recreates mental models that help strengthen neural connections in schemas in long-term memory.
-
An EVENT is what is happening OUTSIDE the student. The MENTAL MODEL is what students construct IN their brain to model—in some sense, understand—the external event with their conscious working memory.
-
Online teaching can give us a great platform to encapsulate key concepts by turning them into tightly scripted events that are easier to retrieve.
References
-
See references for Video 3C-1 and below:
-
Babichev, A and Dabaghian, YA. “Topological schemas of memory spaces.” Frontiers in computational neuroscience 12, (2018): 27.
-
Franklin, NT, et al. “Structured event memory: A neuro-symbolic model of event cognition.” Psychological Review 127, no. 3 (2020): 327.
-
Ghosh, VE and Gilboa, A. “What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature.” Neuropsychologia 53, (2014): 104-114.
-
Gilboa, A and Marlatte, H. “Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory.” Trends Cogn Sci 21, no. 8 (2017): 618-631.
-
Kurby, CA and Zacks, JM. “Priming of movie content is modulated by event boundaries.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, (2021).
Video 3: “If I told you, I’d have to…” Skirting Around the Complexity of Working Memory and Mental Models
Key Concepts
- The mental model being held in working memory is extremely complex, even for the seeming simplest of events. It can be difficult even for experienced neuroscientists to explain to non-neuroscientists. But thinking in terms of metaphors—the mental model is a flock of birds, while working memory is an octopus, can help.
References
- See references for Video 3C-1 and 3C-2.
Video 4: Driving Home the Idea that Retrieval Practice Helps Solidify Both Simple and Complex Events in Schemas
Key Concepts
-
Even seemingly simple ideas, like a vocabulary word in a foreign language, can involve extraordinary complexities. “Simple” retrieval practice isn’t necessarily so simple!
-
Retrieval practice is useful even for complicated topics—as long as the topics are “graspable” by working memory.
References
-
See references for Video 3C-1 and 3C-2, and
-
Nielsen, M. “Using spaced repetition systems to see through a piece of mathematics.” Cognitive Medium (2019).
Video 5: Cognitive Load—If the Event is Too Complicated, Watch Out!
Key Concepts
-
If an event is too complex, it becomes difficult for working memory to grasp. It is better to break the ideas into smaller “flocks” of thoughts or concepts. This is called “scaffolding” or “chunking.”
-
Declarative information passes through the hippocampus. Procedural information passes through the basal ganglia. The basic process of retrieval practice helps with both declarative and procedural learning.
-
Once information is in long-term memory, it can meld together—consolidate—into larger, cohesive chunks of information that are easy to draw into mind even though they involve complicated ideas.
References
-
See references for Video 3C-1 and 3C-2.
-
To get started on the copious literature related to the importance of autonomy in learning and related issues, see the discussions on autonomy (which extend beyond motor learning) in Wulf, G and Lewthwaite, R. “Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning.” Psychon Bull Rev 23, no. 5 (2016): 1382-1414.
Week 3 part 2
Week 3, Lesson 2: Using & Encouraging Retrieval Practice Apps in Your Courses
Video 6: How to Use Retrieval Practice Apps to Encourage Collaboration
References
- See references for Video 3C-1
Video 7: How to Use a Specific Retrieval Practice App (iDoRecall) in Your Coursera Courses
References
- See references for Video 3C-1.
Video 8: How to Solicit Live, Monitored Retrieval Practice from Every Student in a Class Simultaneously (PearDeck™)
-
PearDeck.com
-
Edwards, Luke, “What is Pear Deck and How Does It Work?” 2021, Tech & Learning.com.
Video 9: Working Memory, Non-Native Speakers, and Barb’s Personal Experience with Retrieval Practice
Key Concepts
-
A typical working memory can hold roughly four pieces of information in mind. But the size of a piece of information depends on the learner’s background expertise in the area—their underlying schema.
-
Be aware that non-native speakers also have the added cognitive load of trying to grasp the information being taught via what is to them a foreign language. Reduce the processing burden of foreign language speakers by enunciating clearly and avoiding slang. (We realize, however, that a little slang can sometimes make such a big difference on native speakers of English that it’s worth the tradeoff.)
-
Retrieval practice is particularly valuable for students with a lesser working memory capacity—it can allow them to excel even in difficult subject areas.
References
-
See references for Video 3C-1 and 3C-2 and:
-
Agarwal, PK, et al. “Benefits from retrieval practice are greater for students with lower working memory capacity.” Memory 25, no. 6 (2017): 764-771.
-
Sali, AW and Egner, T. “Declarative and procedural working memory updating processes are mutually facilitative.” Atten Percept Psychophys 82, no. 4 (2020): 1858-1871.
Week 4
Week 4 part 1
Week 4, Lesson 1: Focus, Suspense, and Creativity
Key Concepts, References, and Readings
Video 1: “Focused” versus “Diffuse” Thinking—Setting the Stage for Their Relevance to Online Teaching
Key Concepts
-
The brain has two different ways of functioning—in “focused” or in “diffuse” modes. Focused mode has our tight attention. Diffuse mode is more relaxed, and is associated with mind-wandering, day-dreaming, and creativity.
-
Your online teaching can make intelligent use of these two different modes to help students be more engaged and creative.
References
-
Oakley B, Rogowsky B, Sejnowski T. Uncommon Sense Teaching: Penguin Random House; 2021. See especially Chapter 4 for a discussion and references related to focused (“task positive”) and diffuse (“task negative,” or “default mode network”) ways of thinking.
-
Hoogman, M, et al. “Creativity and ADHD: A review of behavioral studies, the effect of psychostimulants and neural underpinnings.” Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 119, (2020): 66-85.
-
Rau, MA and Herder, T. “Under which conditions are physical versus virtual representations effective? Contrasting conceptual and embodied mechanisms of learning.” Journal of Educational Psychology, (2021). This paper provides background in conceptual salience theory: “Building on working memory theory, conceptual salience theory assumes that students have to explicitly attend to information in order to load it into working memory (Mayer, 2009; Miller, 1956). Representations can be designed so that they attract students’ attention (Mayer & Moreno, 1998; Schnotz, 2005)…. Conceptual salience theory explains the concept-dependency of findings from earlier studies by proposing that the mode that makes a concept more salient yields higher learning gains for that concept (Olympiou & Zacharia, 2012).”
Video 2: Attention—How to Get It, and Why You Want to Sometimes Lose It
Key Concepts
-
Students pay attention because of either top-down conscious will power, or bottom-up automatic attention because of sensory input like a sound or movement. Online learning allows for particularly creative approaches to attracting attention by using bottom-up approaches.
-
Breakout and active sessions where students interact with one another during synchronous online sessions can allow for momentary “diffuse mode” breaks, so they return to focused attention with a fresher perspective.
-
A benefit of asynchronous teaching—that is, watching a video—is that if a student might catch themselves mind-wandering, they can stop the video and go back to where they left off.
References
-
See references from Week 2 Video 4
-
Hutson, J. P., et al. (2022). “Narrative comprehension guides eye movements in the absence of motion.” Cogn Sci 46(5): e13131.
-
Oakley B, Rogowsky B, Sejnowski T. Uncommon Sense Teaching: Penguin Random House; 2021. See especially the material on focused and diffuse modes (“task positive” and “task negative” or “default mode network”) in Chapter 4.
-
Oakley, BA and Sejnowski, TJ. “What we learned from creating one of the world’s most popular MOOCs.” npj Science of Learning 4, Article 7 (2019): 1-7. (See the many references within the paper.)
4C-3: Why Editing Your Own Videos Can Be a Good Idea
Key Concepts
- Experienced academic video editors are not necessarily as good as you can be at creating good educational videos.
References
-
See references from Week 2 Video 4, especially Guo, PJ, et al. “How video production affects student engagement: an empirical study of MOOC videos.” In Proceedings of the First ACM Conference on Learning@Scale Conference, 41-50. Atlanta, GA USA, 2014.
-
Oakley, BA and Sejnowski, TJ. “What we learned from creating one of the world’s most popular MOOCs.” npj Science of Learning 4, Article 7 (2019): 1-7.
4C-4: It’s Good to Leave Them Hanging! The Value of Suspense
Key Concepts
- Suspense and “hooks” in teaching can help suppress the diffuse mode and prevent mind-wandering. This can naturally help students to keep their focus on what is being learned.
References
-
Bezdek, MA and Gerrig, RJ. “When narrative transportation narrows attention: Changes in attentional focus during suspenseful film viewing.” Media Psychology 20, no. 1 (2016): 60-89.
-
Bezdek, MA, et al. “Neural evidence that suspense narrows attentional focus.” Neuroscience 303, (2015): 338-345.
4C-5: Humor Does NOT Mean Being a Comedian
Key Concepts
-
Even a few seconds of diffuse mode relaxing break during class can help students return to focus refreshed—as for example, when students might momentarily turn to work together, or you tell a joke.
-
Sporadic use of humor is especially helpful in keeping students engaged in online teaching.
References
-
Amir, O, et al. “Ha Ha! Versus Aha! A direct comparison of humor to nonhumorous insight for determining the neural correlates of mirth.” Cerebral Cortex, (2013): 1405-1413.
-
Banas, JA, et al. “A review of humor in educational settings: Four decades of research.” Communication Education 60, no. 1 (2011): 115-144.
-
Bolkan, S, et al. “Humor in the classroom: The effects of integrated humor on student learning.” Communication Education 67, no. 2 (2018): 144-164.
-
Chan, YC, et al. “Appreciation of different styles of humor: An fMRI study.” Sci Rep 8, no. 1 (2018): 15649.
-
Garner, RL. “Humor in pedagogy: How ha-ha can lead to aha!” College Teaching 54, no. 1 (2006): 177-180.
-
Hackathorn, J, et al. “All kidding aside: Humor increases learning at knowledge and comprehension levels.” Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning 11, no. 4 (2011): 116-123.
-
Hu, DL, et al. “Humour Applied to STEM Education.” Systems Research and Behavioral Science, (2016).
-
Jeder, D. “Implications of using humor in the classroom.” Procedia Soc Behav Sci 180, (2015): 828-833.
-
McGhee, P. “Humor in the ECE classroom: A neglected form of play whose time has come.” In Research on Young Children’s Humor, 83-106, 2019.
-
Mobbs, D, et al. “Humor modulates the mesolimbic reward centers.” Neuron 40, no. 5 (2003): 1041-1048.
-
Nienaber, K, et al. “The funny thing is, instructor humor style affects likelihood of student engagement.” Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning 19, no. 5 (2019): 53-60.
4C-6: Creating a Social Partnership
Key Concepts
-
Seeing an instructor looking, smiling, and speaking “as if” a conversational partner was there can create the illusion of social partnership.
-
When you are enthusiastic and passionate, the mirroring effect means your students mirror your behavior.
-
Mirroring of your expressive, passionate enthusiasm can help even unmotivated students to become more motivated.
References
-
Bocian, K, et al. “The mere liking effect: Attitudinal influences on attributions of moral character.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 79, (2018): 9-20.
-
Castro-Alonso, JC, et al. “Effectiveness of multimedia pedagogical agents predicted by diverse theories: A meta-analysis.” Educational Psychology Review 33, no. 3 (2021): 989-1015.
-
Coles, NA, et al. “Fact or artifact? Demand characteristics and participants’ beliefs can moderate, but do not fully account for, the effects of facial feedback on emotional experience.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (2022).
-
Hoffner, Cynthia A, and Bradley J Bond. “Parasocial relationships, social media, & well-being.” Current Opinion in Psychology (2022): 101306.
-
Lawson, AP and Mayer, RE. “Does the emotional stance of human and virtual instructors in instructional videos affect learning processes and outcomes?” Contemporary Educational Psychology 70, (2022): 102080.
-
Hume, A. “How to use your voice when teaching online.” LinkedIn (2020).
-
Kokoç, M, et al. “Effects of sustained attention and video lecture types on learning performances.” Educational Technology Research and Development 68, no. 6 (2020): 3015-3039.
-
Larsen, R. J., Kasimatis, M., & Frey, K. (1992). “Facilitating the furrowed brow: An unobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis applied to unpleasant affect.” Cognition & Emotion, 6, 321–38.
-
Martin, & Bolliger, D. U. (2018). “Engagement matters: Student perceptions on the importance of engagement strategies in the online learning environment.” Journal of Asynchronous Learning Networks (JALN), 22(1), 205-222.
-
Pisano, C, et al. “Botulinum toxins: Beauty, psychology, and mood in the cosmetic patient.” In Essential Psychiatry for the Aesthetic Practitioner, edited by Evan A. Rieder and Richard G. Fried, 125-130: Wiley, 2021.
-
Redcay, E and Schilbach, L. “Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 20, no. 8 (2019): 495-505.
-
Shane, S, et al. “Founder passion, neural engagement and informal investor interest in startup pitches: An fMRI study.” Journal of Business Venturing 35, no. 4 (2020).
-
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). “Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–77.
-
Wang, J, et al. “Converging subjective and psychophysiological measures of cognitive load to study the effects of instructor‐present video.” Mind, Brain, and Education 14, no. 3 (2020): 279-291. This paper notes: “Results suggested the instructor-present video improved learners’ ability to transfer information and was associated with a lower self-reported intrinsic and extraneous load. Event-related changes in theta band activity also indicated lower cognitive load with instructor-present video.”
-
Wang, J, et al. “Does visual attention to the instructor in online video affect learning and learner perceptions? An eye-tracking analysis.” Computers & Education 146, (2020): 103779.
-
Wong, M, et al. “Up close and personal: Examining effects of instructor video presence on student’s sense of connection.” Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, (2021).
-
Wulf, G and Lewthwaite, R. “Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning.” Psychon Bull Rev 23, no. 5 (2016): 1382-1414.
-
Zajonc, RB. “Attitudinal effects of mere exposure.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 9, no. 2p2 (1968): 1.
4C-7: Teleprompters and Giant Frogs
Key Concepts
-
Many instructors can benefit from scripted material read from a teleprompter. If you use a teleprompter:
-
Look away from the camera occasionally, as you would in a natural conversation.
-
Be sure to look toward what you might be pointing to. (You can do this even if you are “picture-in-picture.”)
-
-
If you don’t script your material, you can still benefit from using a list of bullet points to talk from on your teleprompter.
References
- Oakley, BA and Sejnowski, TJ. “What we learned from creating one of the world’s most popular MOOCs.” npj Science of Learning 4, Article 7 (2019): 1-7. (See the many references within the paper.)
4C-8: The Paradox of Self-Focus
Key Concepts
-
“Micro-choking” episodes occur when declarative, conscious thoughts about yourself and how you are feeling and looking intrude on your teaching.
-
By moving your thoughts away from yourself and focusing externally on your camera (that is, your students), you activate the automatic procedural system. You can then find yourself teaching smoothly and naturally, without self-consciousness.
-
Eyebrow flashes, head tilts, and full, natural smiles that also bring wrinkles to the eyes can signal your students that you are friendly and approachable. What student, after all, wants to take a course from an unlikeable instructor?
-
Make sure you are not too close to the camera, so that your stimulating gaze doesn’t become overstimulating.
References
-
Babad, E. “Teachers’ nonverbal behavior and its effects on students.” In The Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education: An Evidence-Based Perspective, 201-261: Springer, 2007.
-
Bailenson, JN. “Nonverbal overload: A theoretical argument for the causes of Zoom fatigue.” Technology, Mind, and Behavior 2, no. 1 (2021).
-
Ratan, R, et al. “Facial appearance dissatisfaction explains differences in Zoom fatigue.” Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 25, no. 2 (2022): 124-129.
-
Schafer, J and Karlins, M. The Like Switch: An Ex-FBI Agent’s Guide to Influencing, Attracting, and Winning People Over. Simon and Schuster, 2015.
-
Wong, M, et al. “Up close and personal: Examining effects of instructor video presence on student’s sense of connection.” Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, (2021).
-
Wulf, G and Lewthwaite, R. “Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning.” Psychon Bull Rev 23, no. 5 (2016): 1382-1414.
Week 4 part 2
Key Concepts, References, and Readings
Video 9: Mental Model and Schema Sharing
Key Concepts
-
Students and teachers have differing underlying schemas—neural frameworks of expertise—in their long-term memories.
-
Teachers’ schemas about their subject are rich and complex. Students’ schemas on the topic of study are naturally very sparse—although students can still follow and grasp, sometimes through the use of metaphor, their teachers’ thoughts (their “mental models”).
-
As students develop their mental models about what is being taught, their thoughts are like flocks of birds that circle and swoop. Students can help guide one another’s neural “flocks” of thoughts when the teacher is inadvertently drawing on schemas that the students do not yet have.
References
-
Czeszumski, A, et al. “Hyperscanning: A valid method to study neural inter-brain underpinnings of social interaction.” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14, (2020): 39.
-
Dikker, S, et al. “Brain-to-brain synchrony tracks real-world dynamic group interactions in the classroom.” Curr Biol 27, no. 9 (2017): 1375-1380.
-
Pan, Y, et al. “Instructor-learner body coupling reflects instruction and learning.” NPJ Sci Learn 7, no. 1 (2022): 15.
-
Oberwelland, E, et al. “Look into my eyes: Investigating joint attention using interactive eye-tracking and fMRI in a developmental sample.” NeuroImage 130, (2016): 248-260. (This paper also mentions the importance of the familiarity of the person who one is interacting with.)
-
Pfeiffer, UJ, et al. “Why we interact: On the functional role of the striatum in the subjective experience of social interaction.” NeuroImage 101, (2014): 124-137.
-
Redcay, E and Schilbach, L. “Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 20, no. 8 (2019): 495-505.
-
Schilbach, L, et al. “Minds made for sharing: Initiating joint attention recruits reward-related neurocircuitry.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, no. 12 (2010): 2702-2715.
-
Xue, H, et al. “Cooperation makes two less-creative individuals turn into a highly-creative pair.” NeuroImage 172, (2018): 527-537.
Video 10: Jolts of Joy
Key Concepts
-
Feeling that another, familiar person is interacting with us activates mirroring and other circuits that make us feel rewarded. This can allow us to find pleasure in learning with others.
-
We need to use caution and balance in our teaching—when collective approaches are emphasized, people can come to desire acceptance by the group over acknowledging truth and empirical evidence. And the larger the collaborative team, the more creativity is reduced.
-
The “synchronicity paradox” relates to the idea that many students crave rich synchronous experience at the same time that they crave the flexibility of asynchronous online learning.
-
Eye gaze can provide for powerful feelings of connection. But for some neurodiverse students, these feelings can become overpowering.
References
See references for Week 4, Video 9: Mental Model and Schema Sharing, as well as the references below.
-
Azoulay, P. “Small-team science is beautiful.” Nature 566, no. 7744 (2019): 330-332.
-
de Lima, DP, et al. “What to expect, and how to improve online discussion forums: the instructors’ perspective.” Journal of Internet Services and Applications 10, no. 1 (2019): 1-15.
-
Denworth, L. “‘Hyperscans show how brains sync as people interact.” Scientific American. (2019).
-
Hadjikhani, N, et al. “Look me in the eyes: Constraining gaze in the eye-region provokes abnormally high subcortical activation in autism.” Sci Rep 7, no. 1, Article 3163 (2017): 1-7.
-
Hone, KS and El Said, GR. “Exploring the factors affecting MOOC retention: A survey study.” Computers & Education 98, (2016): 157-168.
-
Joyner, DA, et al. “The synchronicity paradox in online education.” In Proceedings of the Seventh ACM Conference on Learning @ Scale, 15–24. Virtual Event, USA: Association for Computing Machinery, 2020.
-
Lin, Y, et al. “Seeing meaning even when none may exist: Collectivism increases belief in empty claims.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 122, no. 3 (2022): 351–366.
-
Ragupathi, K. Facilitating Effective Online Discussions: Resource Guide. Centre for Development of Teaching and Learning (CDTL), National University of Singapore (2018). This comprehensive overview also includes a discussion of best practices for discussion forums.
-
Nihalani, PK, et al. “When feedback harms and collaboration helps in computer simulation environments: An expertise reversal effect.” Journal of Educational Psychology 103, no. 4 (2011): 776-785.
-
Wu, L, et al. “Large teams develop and small teams disrupt science and technology.” Nature 566, (2019): 378-382.
Video 11: The Challenge of Discussion Forums
Key Concepts
-
Making discussions mandatory or trying to have novice students give answers and feedback on one another’s questions are not good uses of this environment.
-
To get students started using a discussion forum in smaller classes, encourage students to post their questions, and give bonus points for providing helpful hints in answer.
References
-
Czeszumski, A, et al. “Hyperscanning: A valid method to study neural inter-brain underpinnings of social interaction.” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14, (2020): 39.
-
Hone, KS and El Said, GR. “Exploring the factors affecting MOOC retention: A survey study.” Computers & Education 98, (2016): 157-168.
-
Joyner, D. Teaching at Scale: Improving Access, Outcomes, and Impact Through Digital Instruction: Routledge, 2022.
-
Nihalani, PK, et al. “When feedback harms and collaboration helps in computer simulation environments: An expertise reversal effect.” Journal of Educational Psychology 103, no. 4 (2011): 776-785.
-
Oberwelland, E, et al. “Look into my eyes: Investigating joint attention using interactive eye-tracking and fMRI in a developmental sample.” NeuroImage 130, (2016): 248-260. (This paper also mentions the importance of the familiarity of the person who one is interacting with.)
-
Redcay, E and Schilbach, L. “Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 20, no. 8 (2019): 495-505.
Video 12: Wrap Up: Learning as Therapy—and As Preparation for the Future!
Key Concepts
-
New learning helps new neurons survive and thrive—which helps improve learners’ moods.
-
Online teaching provides a vitally important form of human infrastructure. Your role in building and maintaining this infrastructure can make a tremendous difference for your students, and for society as a whole.
-
Our hope is that the mental models—the key concepts—we’ve been describing in this course have been transferred and transformed within you as they’ve gradually made their way into your schemas of long-term memory.
References
-
Anacker, C and Hen, R. “Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and cognitive flexibility linking memory and mood.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 18, no. 6 (2017): 335-346.
-
de Oliveira, JMD, et al. “Mental health effects prevalence in children and adolescents during the COVID‐19 pandemic: A systematic review.” Worldviews on Evidence‐Based Nursing 19, no. 2 (2022): 130-137.
-
Festini, SB, et al. “The busier the better: greater busyness is associated with better cognition.” Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 8, (2016): 98.
-
Filindassi, V, et al. “Impact of COVID-19 first wave on psychological and psychosocial dimensions: A systematic review.” COVID 2, no. 3 (2022): 273-340.
-
Garagiola, ER, et al. “Adolescent resilience during the COVID-19 pandemic: A Review of the impact of the pandemic on developmental milestones.” Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 7 (2022).
-
Hu, Y-H. “Effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on the online learning behaviors of university students in Taiwan.” Education and Information Technologies 27, no. 1 (2022): 469-491.
-
Shane, S, et al. “Founder passion, neural engagement and informal investor interest in startup pitches: An fMRI study.” Journal of Business Venturing 35, no. 4 (2020). As the authors note “Neural engagement as defined by ‘the extent to which an observer’s brain becomes engrossed in a stimulus.’” “…high displays of passion recruits the brain in a way that increases fixation on the stimulus, which is the pitch. And because higher neural engagement increases focus and attention to the content and overrides distractions, we theorize that more engaged brains are more likely to meaningfully evaluate pitches, which should result in more favorable investor assessments. We argue that neural engagement partially mediates the displayed passion-investor interest relationship: Displays of passion trigger heightened engagement that, in turn, makes investors more likely to invest.” As it is with investors is as it can be with inducing students to passionately decide to engage and invest in learning!
-
Shors, T. J. (2014). “The adult brain makes new neurons, and effortful learning keeps them alive.” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 311–318.
-
Specter, M. “Rethinking the brain: How the songs of canaries upset a fundamental principle of science.” The New Yorker, 2001, 42-53. This article describes some of the original controversy regarding whether neurogenesis actually occurred in humans.
-
Yeager, A. “What do new neurons in the brains of adults actually do?” The Scientist (2020). (This is a wonderfully readable overview article.)